Jump to content

Chimpanzee

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Chimpanzees[1]
Temporal range: 4–0 Ma
Common chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)
Scientific classification
Kingdom: Animalia
Phylum: Chordata
Class: Mammalia
Order: Primates
Family: Hominidae
Subfamily: Homininae
Tribe: Panini
Genus: ''Pan''
Oken, 1816
Type species
Pan troglodytes
Species

Pan troglodytes
Pan paniscus

Distribution of Pan troglodytes (common chimpanzee) and Pan paniscus (bonobo, in red)
Synonyms

Troglodytes E. Geoffroy, 1812 (preoccupied)
Mimetes Leach, 1820 (preoccupied)
Theranthropus Brookes, 1828
Chimpansee Voight, 1831
Anthropopithecus Blainville, 1838
Hylanthropus Gloger, 1841
Pseudanthropus Reichenbach, 1862
Engeco Haeckel, 1866
Fsihego DePauw, 1905

Chimpanzees, sometimes colloquially chimps, are two extant hominid species of apes in the genus Pan. The Congo River divides the native habitats of the two species:[2]

Chimpanzees are members of the family Hominidae, along with humans, gorillas, and orangutans. Chimpanzees split from the human branch of the family about four to six million years ago. Chimpanzees are the closest living relatives to humans, being members of the tribe Hominini (along with extinct species of subtribe Hominina). Chimpanzees are the only known members of the subtribe Panina. The two Pan species split only about one million years ago.

Name

[edit]

The first use of the name "Chimpanze" is recorded in The London Magazine in 1738,[3] glossed as meaning "Mockman" in a language of "the Angolans" (apparently from a Bantu language, reportedly modern Vili (Civili), a Zone H Bantu language, has the comparable ci-mpenzi[4]). The spelling Chimpanzee is found in a 1758 supplement to Chamber's Cyclopædia.[5] The colloquialism "chimp" was most likely coined some time in the late 1870s.[6]

The common chimpanzee was named Simia troglodytes by Johann Friedrich Blumenbach in 1776. The species name troglodytes is a reference to the Troglodytae (literally "cave-goers"), an African people described by Greco-Roman geographers. Blumenbach first used it in his De generis humani varietate nativa liber ("[Book] on the natural varieties of the human genus") in 1776,[7][8] Linnaeus 1758 had already used Homo troglodytes for a hypothetical mixture of human and orang utan.[9]

The genus name Pan was first introduced by Lorenz Oken in 1816. An alternative Theranthropus was suggested by Brookes 1828 and Chimpansee by Voigt 1831. Troglodytes was not available, as it had been given as the name of a genus of wren (Troglodytidae) in 1809. The International Commission on Zoological Nomenclature adopted Pan as the only official name of the genus in 1895.[9] The name is a reference to Pan, the Greek god of nature and wilderness.[10] The Bonobo, also known as the "pygmy chimpanzee", was given the species name of paniscus by Ernst Schwarz (1929), a diminutive of the theonym Pan.[11]

Evolutionary history

[edit]

Evolutionary relationship

[edit]

The genus Pan is part of the subfamily Homininae, to which humans also belong. These two species are the closest living evolutionary relatives to humans, sharing a common ancestor with humans about four to six million years ago.[12] Research by Mary-Claire King in 1973 found 99% identical DNA between human beings and chimpanzees,[13] although research since has modified that finding to about 94%[14] commonality, with some of the difference occurring in noncoding DNA. P. troglodytes and P. paniscus have been proposed to belong with H. sapiens in the genus Homo, rather than in Pan; e.g., by J. Diamond in his book, wherein he refers to man as The Third Chimpanzee. Among the arguments in favor of this reclassification is that other species have been reclassified to belong to the same genus because of less genetic similarity than that between humans and chimpanzees.

Taxonomy of genus Pan[1] Phylogeny of superfamily Hominoidea[15]: Fig. 4 
 Hominoidea

Template:Cladex

Fossils

[edit]

Though many human fossils have been found, chimpanzee fossils were not described until 2005. Existing chimpanzee populations in West and Central Africa are separate from the major human fossil sites in East Africa; however, chimpanzee fossils have been reported from Kenya, indicating that both humans and members of the Pan clade were present in the East African Rift Valley during the Middle Pleistocene.[16]

Anatomy and physiology

[edit]
Human and chimp skulls and brains (not to scale), as illustrated in Gervais' Histoire naturelle des mammifères
The chimpanzee's brain on the left and the man's brain on the right have been scaled to the same size to show the relative proportions of their parts. These drawings were in a book made in 1904 by Thomas Henry Huxley.[17]

The male common chimp stands up to 1.7 m (5.6 ft) high and weighs as much as 70 kg (150 lb); the female is somewhat smaller. The common chimp’s long arms, when extended, span one and a half times the body’s height. A chimpanzee's arms are longer than its legs.[18] The bonobo is slightly shorter and thinner than the common chimpanzee but has longer limbs. In trees, both species climb with their long, powerful arms; on the ground, chimpanzees usually knuckle-walk, or walk on all fours, clenching their fists and supporting themselves on the knuckles thereof. Chimpanzee feet are better suited for walking than are those of the orangutan because the chimp has broader soles and shorter toes.

Both the common chimpanzee and bonobo can walk upright on two legs when carrying objects with their hands and arms. The bonobo has proportionately longer upper limbs and more often walks upright than does the common chimpanzee. The coat is dark; the face, fingers, palms of the hands, and soles of the feet, hairless; the chimp, tailless. The exposed skin of the face, hands and feet varies from pink to very dark in both species but is generally lighter in younger individuals, darkening as maturity is reached. A University of Chicago Medical Centre study has found significant genetic differences between chimpanzee populations.[19] A bony shelf over the eyes gives the forehead a receding appearance, and the nose is flat. Although the jaws protrude, the lips are thrust out only when a chimp pouts.

The brain of a chimpanzee has been measured at ~337 cc,[20][21] ~393 cc,[22] with a general range of 282–500 cc.[23] Human brains, in contrast, have been measured as being three times larger, variously reported volumes include ~1,299 cc,[20][21] ~1,158 cc,[22] and averages of ~1330 cc.[24][25][26][27][28][29][30][31]

Chimpanzee testicles are unusually large for their body size, with a combined weight of about 4 oz (110 g) compared to a gorilla's 1 oz (28 g) or a human's 1.5 ounces (43 g). This relatively great size is generally attributed to sperm competition due to the polyandrous nature of chimpanzee mating behavior.[32] Chimpanzees reach puberty between the age of eight and ten years[citation needed] and one study estimates that they live an average of 35 years (33 years for males, and 37 years for females) in the wild,[33] but some have lived longer than 60 years in captivity.[citation needed]

Behavior

[edit]
(video) Female chimpanzee at Tobu Zoo in Saitama, Japan.
Bonobo

Anatomical differences between the common chimpanzee and the bonobo are slight, but sexual and social behaviors are markedly different. The common chimpanzee has an omnivorous diet, a troop hunting culture based on beta males led by an alpha male, and highly complex social relationships. The bonobo, on the other hand, has a mostly frugivorous diet and an egalitarian, nonviolent, matriarchal, sexually receptive behavior.[34] Bonobos frequently have sex, sometimes to help prevent and resolve conflicts. Different groups of chimpanzees also have different cultural behavior with preferences for types of tools.[35] The common chimpanzee tends to display greater aggression than does the bonobo.[36] The average, captive chimpanzee sleeps 9.7 hours per day.[37]

Social structure

[edit]
Common chimpanzees in Gombe Stream National Park

Chimpanzees live in large multi-male and multi-female social groups, which are called communities. Within a community, the position of an individual and the influence the individual has on others dictates a definite social hierarchy. Chimpanzees live in a leaner hierarchy wherein more than one individual may be dominant enough to dominate other members of lower rank. Typically, a dominant male is referred to as the alpha male. The alpha male is the highest-ranking male that controls the group and maintains order during disputes. In chimpanzee society, the 'dominant male' sometimes is not the largest or strongest male but rather the most manipulative and political male that can influence the goings on within a group. Male chimpanzees typically attain dominance by cultivating allies who will support that individual during future ambitions for power. The alpha male regularly displays by puffing his normally slim coat up to increase view size and charge to seem as threatening and as powerful as possible; this behavior serves to intimidate other members and thereby maintain power and authority, and it may be fundamental to the alpha male's holding on to his status. Lower-ranking chimpanzees will show respect by submissively gesturing in body language or reaching out their hands while grunting. Female chimpanzees will show deference to the alpha male by presenting their hindquarters.

Female chimpanzees also have a hierarchy, which is influenced by the position of a female individual within a group. In some chimpanzee communities, the young females may inherit high status from a high-ranking mother. Dominant females will also ally to dominate lower-ranking females: whereas males mainly seek dominant status for its associated mating privileges and sometimes violent domination of subordinates, females seek dominant status to acquire such resources as food. High-ranking females often have first access. Both genders acquire dominant status to improve social standing within a group.

Community female acceptance is necessary for alpha male status; females must ensure that their group visits places that supply them with enough food. A group of dominant females will sometimes oust an alpha male which is not to their preference and back another male, in whom they see potential for leading the group as a successful alpha male.

Intelligence

[edit]
Diagram of brain – topography of the main groups of foci in the motor field of chimpanzee

Chimpanzees make tools and use them to acquire foods and for social displays; they have sophisticated hunting strategies requiring cooperation, influence and rank; they are status conscious, manipulative and capable of deception; they can learn to use symbols and understand aspects of human language including some relational syntax, concepts of number and numerical sequence;[38] and they are capable of spontaneous planning for a future state or event.[39]

Tool use

[edit]
Common chimpanzee using a stick

In October 1960, Jane Goodall observed the use of tools among chimpanzees. Recent research indicates chimpanzee stone tool use dates to at least 4,300 years ago.[40] Chimpanzee tool usage includes digging into termite mounds with a large stick tool, and then using a small stick that has been altered to "fish" the termites out.[41] There have been occasional unsubstantiated or controversial reports of Chimpanzees using rocks or sticks as weapons.[42] A recent study claimed to reveal the use of spears, which common chimpanzees in Senegal sharpen with their teeth and use to stab and pry Senegal bushbabies out of small holes in trees.[43][44] Chimpanzees are also known to use stones as anvils and hammers in order to break open nuts.[45] Before the discovery of tool use in chimps, humans were believed to be the only species to make and use tools, but several other tool-using species are now known.[46][47]

Nest-building

[edit]

Nest-building, sometimes considered as tool use, is seen in chimpanzees which construct arboreal night nests by lacing together branches from one or more trees. It forms an important part of behavior, especially in the case of mothers who teach this trait to infants. Nests consist of a mattress, supported on a strong foundation, and lined above with soft leaves and twigs, and are built in trees with a minimum diameter of 5 metres (16 ft) and may be located at a height of 3 to 45 metres (9.8 to 147.6 ft). Both day and night nests are built; they may be located in groups.[48] A study in 2014 found that the Muhimbi tree is favoured for nest building by chimpanzees in Uganda due to its physical properties, such as bending strength, inter-node distance, and leaf surface area.[49]

Altruism and emotivity

[edit]
Chimpanzee mother and baby

Studies have shown chimpanzees engage in apparently altruistic behavior within groups.[50][51] Some researchers have said chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of unrelated group members,[52] but a more recent study of wild chimpanzees found that both male and female adults would adopt orphaned young of their group. Also, different groups sometimes share food, form coalitions, and cooperate in hunting and border patrolling.[53] Sometimes chimpanzees have adopted young that come from unrelated groups. And in some rare cases, even male chimps have been shown to take care of abandoned infant chimps of an unrelated group, though in most cases they would kill the infant.[citation needed]

According to James W. Harrod, evidence for chimpanzee emotivity includes display of mourning, "incipient romantic love", rain dances, appreciation of natural beauty such as a sunset over a lake, curiosity and respect towards wildlife (such as the python, which is neither a threat nor a food source to chimpanzees), altruism toward other species (such as feeding turtles) and animism, or "pretend play", in chimps cradling and grooming rocks or sticks.[54]

Communication

[edit]

Chimps communicate in a manner similar to human nonverbal communication, using vocalizations, hand gestures, and facial expressions. There is even some evidence that they can recreate human speech.[55] Research into the chimpanzee brain has revealed chimp communication activates an area of the chimp brain in the same position as Broca's area, a language center in the human brain.[56]

There is some debate as to whether chimpanzees have the ability to express hierarchical ideas in language. Studies have found that chimps are capable of learning a limited set of sign language symbols, which they can use to communicate with human trainers. However, it is clear that there are distinct limits to the complexity of knowledge structures with which chimps are capable of dealing[citation needed]. The sentences that they can express are limited to specific simple noun-verb sequences and are not capable of the indefinite expansion of complexity characteristic of humans.

Aggression

[edit]

Adult common chimpanzees, particularly males, can be very aggressive. They are highly territorial and are known to kill other chimps.[57]

Common chimpanzee with hunted bushbuck on a tree in Gombe Stream National Park

Hunting

[edit]

Chimpanzees also engage in targeted hunting of lower-order primates such as the red colobus[58] and bush babies,[59][60] and use the meat from these kills as a "social tool" within their community.[61]

Puzzle solving

[edit]

In February 2013, a study found chimpanzees solve puzzles for entertainment.[62]

History

[edit]
62-year-old chimpanzee
Gregoire: 62-year-old chimpanzee

Chimps, as well as other apes, had also been purported to have been known to ancient writers, but mainly as myths and legends on the edge of European and Near Eastern societal consciousness. Apes are mentioned variously by Aristotle. The English word ape translates Hebrew qőf in English translations of the Bible (1 Kings 10:22), but the word may refer to a monkey rather than an ape proper.

The diary of Portuguese explorer Duarte Pacheco Pereira (1506), preserved in the Portuguese National Archive (Torre do Tombo), is probably the first written document to acknowledge chimpanzees built their own rudimentary tools. The first of these early transcontinental chimpanzees came from Angola and were presented as a gift to Frederick Henry, Prince of Orange in 1640, and were followed by a few of its brethren over the next several years. Scientists described these first chimpanzees as "pygmies", and noted the animals' distinct similarities to humans. The next two decades, a number of the creatures were imported into Europe, mainly acquired by various zoological gardens as entertainment for visitors.

Hugo Rheinhold's Affe mit Schädel ("Ape with skull").

Darwin's theory of natural selection (published in 1859) spurred scientific interest in chimpanzees, as in much of life science, leading eventually to numerous studies of the animals in the wild and captivity. The observers of chimpanzees at the time were mainly interested in behavior as it related to that of humans. This was less strictly and disinterestedly scientific than it might sound, with much attention being focused on whether or not the animals had traits that could be considered 'good'; the intelligence of chimpanzees was often significantly exaggerated, as immortalized in Hugo Rheinhold's Affe mit Schädel (see image, left). By the end of the 19th century, chimpanzees remained very much a mystery to humans, with very little factual scientific information available.

In the 20th century, a new age of scientific research into chimpanzee behavior began. Before 1960, almost nothing was known about chimpanzee behavior in their natural habitats. In July of that year, Jane Goodall set out to Tanzania's Gombe forest to live among the chimpanzees, where she primarily studied the members of the Kasakela chimpanzee community. Her discovery that chimpanzees made and used tools was groundbreaking, as humans were previously believed to be the only species to do so. The most progressive early studies on chimpanzees were spearheaded primarily by Wolfgang Köhler and Robert Yerkes, both of whom were renowned psychologists. Both men and their colleagues established laboratory studies of chimpanzees focused specifically on learning about the intellectual abilities of chimpanzees, particularly problem-solving. This typically involved basic, practical tests on laboratory chimpanzees, which required a fairly high intellectual capacity (such as how to solve the problem of acquiring an out-of-reach banana). Notably, Yerkes also made extensive observations of chimpanzees in the wild which added tremendously to the scientific understanding of chimpanzees and their behavior. Yerkes studied chimpanzees until World War II, while Köhler concluded five years of study and published his famous Mentality of Apes in 1925 (which is coincidentally when Yerkes began his analyses), eventually concluding, "chimpanzees manifest intelligent behaviour of the general kind familiar in human beings ... a type of behaviour which counts as specifically human" (1925).[63]

Chimpanzee at the Los Angeles Zoo

The August 2008 issue of the American Journal of Primatology reported results of a year-long study of chimpanzees in Tanzania’s Mahale Mountains National Park, which produced evidence of chimpanzees becoming sick from viral infectious diseases they have likely contracted from humans. Molecular, microscopic and epidemiological investigations demonstrated the chimpanzees living at Mahale Mountains National Park have been suffering from a respiratory disease that is likely caused by a variant of a human paramyxovirus.[64]

Research and study

[edit]

Template:See also As of November 2007, about 1,300 chimpanzees were housed in 10 U.S. laboratories (out of 3,000 great apes living in captivity there), either wild-caught, or acquired from circuses, animal trainers, or zoos.[65] Most of the labs either conduct or make the chimps available for invasive research,[66] defined as "inoculation with an infectious agent, surgery or biopsy conducted for the sake of research and not for the sake of the chimpanzee, and/or drug testing".[67] Two federally funded laboratories use chimps: the Yerkes National Primate Research Center at Emory University in Atlanta, Georgia, and the Southwest National Primate Center in San Antonio, Texas.[68] Five hundred chimps have been retired from laboratory use in the U.S. and live in animal sanctuaries in the U.S. or Canada.[66]

Ham the Astrochimp before being inserted into the Mercury-Redstone 2 capsule in 1961

Chimpanzees used in biomedical research tend to be used repeatedly over decades, rather than used and killed as with most laboratory animals. Some individual chimps currently in U.S. laboratories have been used in experiments for over 40 years.[69] According to Project R&R, a campaign to release chimps held in U.S. labs—run by the New England Anti-Vivisection Society in conjunction with Jane Goodall and other primate researchers—the oldest known chimp in a U.S. lab is Wenka, which was born in a laboratory in Florida on May 21, 1954.[70] She was removed from her mother on the day of birth to be used in a vision experiment that lasted 17 months, then sold as a pet to a family in North Carolina. She was returned to the Yerkes National Primate Research Center in 1957 when she became too big to handle. Since then, she has given birth six times, and has been the subject of research into alcohol use, oral contraceptives, aging, and cognitive studies.[71]

With the publication of the chimpanzee genome, plans to increase the use of chimps in labs are reportedly increasing, with some scientists arguing the federal moratorium on breeding chimps for research should be lifted.[68][72] A five-year moratorium was imposed by the U.S. National Institutes of Health in 1996, because too many chimps had been bred for HIV research, and it has been extended annually since 2001.[68]

Other researchers argue chimps are unique animals and either should not be used in research, or should be treated differently. Pascal Gagneux, an evolutionary biologist and primate expert at the University of California, San Diego, argues, given chimpanzees' sense of self, tool use, and genetic similarity to human beings, studies using chimps should follow the ethical guidelines used for human subjects unable to give consent.[68] Also, a recent study suggests chimpanzees which are retired from labs exhibit a form of posttraumatic stress disorder.[73] Stuart Zola, director of the Yerkes National Primate Research Laboratory, disagrees. He told National Geographic: "I don't think we should make a distinction between our obligation to treat humanely any species, whether it's a rat or a monkey or a chimpanzee. No matter how much we may wish it, chimps are not human."[68]

An increasing number of governments are enacting a great ape research ban forbidding the use of chimpanzees and other great apes in research or toxicology testing.[74] As of 2006, Austria, New Zealand, the Netherlands, Sweden, and the UK had introduced such bans.[75]

Studies of language

[edit]
Side profile of a chimpanzee

Scientists have long been fascinated with the studies of language, believing it to be a unique human cognitive ability. To test this hypothesis, scientists have attempted to teach human language to several species of great apes. One early attempt by Allen and Beatrix Gardner in the 1960s involved spending 51 months teaching American Sign Language (ASL) to a chimpanzee named Washoe. The Gardners reported Washoe learned 151 signs, and she had spontaneously taught them to other chimpanzees.[76] Over a longer period of time, Washoe learned over 800 signs.[77]

Debate is ongoing among some scientists (such as David Premack), about nonhuman great apes' ability to learn language. Since the early reports on Washoe, numerous other studies have been conducted, with varying levels of success,[78] including one involving a chimpanzee named, in parody, Nim Chimpsky, trained by Herbert Terrace of Columbia University. Although his initial reports were quite positive, in November 1979, Terrace and his team (including psycholinguist Thomas Bever) re-evaluated the videotapes of Nim with his trainers, analyzing them frame by frame for signs, as well as for exact context (what was happening both before and after Nim’s signs). In the reanalysis, Terrace and Bever concluded Nim's utterances could be explained merely as prompting on the part of the experimenters, as well as mistakes in reporting the data. "Much of the apes’ behavior is pure drill," he said. "Language still stands as an important definition of the human species." In this reversal, Terrace now argued Nim's use of ASL was not like human language acquisition. Nim never initiated conversations himself, rarely introduced new words, and simply imitated what the humans did. More importantly, Nim's word strings varied in their ordering, suggesting that he was incapable of syntax. Nim’s sentences also did not grow in length, unlike human children whose vocabulary and sentence length show a strong positive correlation.[79]

Memory

[edit]

A 30-year study at Kyoto University’s Primate Research Institute has shown chimps are able to learn to recognize the numbers 1 through 9 and their values. The chimps further show an aptitude for photographic memory, demonstrated in experiments in which the jumbled digits are flashed onto a computer screen for less than a quarter of a second, after which the chimp, Ayumu, is able to correctly and quickly point to the positions where they appeared in ascending order. The same experiment was failed by human world memory champion Ben Pridmore on most attempts.[80]

Laughter in apes

[edit]
Young chimpanzees
Young chimpanzees playing

Laughter might not be confined or unique to humans. The differences between chimpanzee and human laughter may be the result of adaptations that have evolved to enable human speech. Self-awareness of one's situation as seen in the mirror test, or the ability to identify with another's predicament (see mirror neurons), are prerequisites for laughter,[citation needed] so animals may be laughing in the same way as humans do.

Chimpanzees, gorillas, and orangutans show laughter-like vocalizations in response to physical contact, such as wrestling, play-chasing, or tickling. This is documented in wild and captive chimpanzees. Common chimpanzee laughter is not readily recognizable to humans as such, because it is generated by alternating inhalations and exhalations that sound more like breathing and panting. Instances in which nonhuman primates have expressed joy have been reported. One study analyzed and recorded sounds made by human babies and bonobos when tickled. Although the bonobo's laugh was a higher frequency, the laugh followed a pattern similar to that of human babies and included similar facial expressions. Humans and chimpanzees share similar ticklish areas of the body, such as the armpits and belly. The enjoyment of tickling in chimpanzees does not diminish with age.[81] Template:See also

Chimpanzees as pets

[edit]

Template:See also Chimpanzees have traditionally been kept as pets in a few African villages, especially in the Democratic Republic of Congo. In Virunga National Park in the east of the country, the park authorities regularly confiscate chimpanzees from people keeping them as pets.[82]

Chimpanzees are popular as wild pets in many areas despite their strength, aggression, and wild nature. Even in areas where keeping non-human primates as pets are illegal, the exotic pet trade continues to prosper and some people keep chimpanzees as pets mistakenly believing that they will bond with them for life. As they grow, so do their strength and aggression; some owners and others interacting with the animals have lost fingers and suffered severe facial damage among other injuries sustained in attacks. In addition to the animals' hostile potential and strength well beyond any human being, chimpanzees physically mature a lot more proportionally than do human beings, and even among the most cleanly and well-organized of housekeepers, maintaining cleanliness and control of chimpanzees is physically demanding to the point that it is impossible for humans to control, especially due to the animals' strength and aggression.[83]

[edit]

Template:See also

Chimpanzees have been commonly stereotyped in popular culture, where they are most often cast in standardized roles as childlike companions, sidekicks or clowns.[84] They are especially suited for the latter role on account of their prominent facial features, long limbs and fast movements, which humans often find amusing. Accordingly, entertainment acts featuring chimpanzees dressed up as humans have been traditional staples of circuses and stage shows.[84]

In the age of television, a new genre of chimp act emerged in the United States: series whose cast consisted entirely of chimpanzees dressed as humans and "speaking" lines dubbed by human actors.[84] These shows, examples of which include Lancelot Link, Secret Chimp in the 1970s or The Chimp Channel in the 1990s, relied on the novelty of their ape cast to make their timeworn, low comedy gags funny.[84] Their chimpanzee "actors" were as interchangeable as the apes in a circus act, being amusing as chimpanzees and not as individuals.[84] Animal rights groups have urged a stop to this practice, considering it animal abuse.[85]

When chimpanzees appear in other TV shows, they generally do so as comic relief sidekicks to humans. In that role, for instance, J. Fred Muggs appeared with Today Show host Dave Garroway in the 1950s, Judy on Daktari in the 1960s or Darwin on The Wild Thornberrys in the 1990s.[84] In contrast to the fictional depictions of other animals, such as dogs (as in Lassie), dolphins (Flipper), horses (The Black Stallion) or even other great apes (King Kong), chimpanzee characters and actions are rarely relevant to the plot.[84]

Portrayals in science fiction

[edit]

The rare depictions of chimpanzees as individuals rather than stock characters, and as central rather than incidental to the plot[84] are generally found in works of science fiction. Robert A. Heinlein's short story "Jerry Was a Man" (1947) centers on a genetically enhanced chimpanzee suing for better treatment. The 1972 film Conquest of the Planet of the Apes, the third sequel of Planet of the Apes, portrays a futuristic revolt of enslaved apes led by the only talking chimpanzee, Caesar, against their human masters.[84] This concept was revisited in the 2011 film Rise of the Planet of the Apes, again with a chimpanzee protagonist named Caesar. Another short story, "The Pope of the Chimps" by Robert Silverberg, set in the present day, shows the development of the first signs of religiosity in a group of chimpanzees, much to the surprise of the humans observing them. David Brin's Uplift novels present a future in which humans have "uplifted" chimpanzees (and certain other species) with human-level sapience.

See also

[edit]
  • [[Archivo:
  1. REDIRECCIÓN Plantilla:Iconos|20px|Ver el portal sobre Primates]] Portal:Primates. Contenido relacionado con Mammals.

Notes

[edit]
  1. ^ a b Template:MSW3 Groves
  2. ^ Shefferly, N. (2005). "Pan troglodytes". Animal Diversity Web (University of Michigan Museum of Zoology). Retrieved 2007-08-11.
  3. ^ The London Magazine 465, September 1738. "A most surprizing creature is brought over in the Speaker, just arrived from Carolina, that was taken in a wood at Guinea. She is the Female of the Creature which the Angolans call Chimpanze, or the Mockman." (cited after OED)
  4. ^ "chimpanzee" in American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language, Fifth Edition, Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company, 2011.
  5. ^ "Chimpanzee, the name of an Angolan animal..In the year 1738, we had one of these creatures brought over into England." (cited after OED)
  6. ^ "Online Etymology Dictionary". Retrieved 2015-03-12. "chimp definition | Dictionary.com". Dictionary.reference.com. Retrieved 2009-06-06.
  7. ^ p. 37 in Blumenbach, J. F. 1776. De generis hvmani varietate nativa liber. Cvm figvris aeri incisis. – pp. [1], 1–100, [1], Tab. I-II [= 1–2]. Goettingae. (Vandenhoeck).
  8. ^ AnimalBase species taxon summary for troglodytes Blumenbach, 1776 described in Simia, version 11 June 2011
  9. ^ a b Tubbs, P.K. 1985: OPINION 1368. THE GENERIC NAMES PAN AND PANTHERA (MAMMALIA, CARNIVORA): AVAILABLE AS FROM OKEN, 1816. Bulletin of zoological nomenclature, 42: 365-370. ISSN: 0007-5167 Internet Archive BHL BioStor [corrigendum in Bulletin of zoological nomenclature, 45: 304. (1988) Internet Archive BHL]
  10. ^ Bo Beolens, Michael Watkins, Michael Grayson, The Eponym Dictionary of Mammals, JHU Press, 2009.
  11. ^ Charlton T. Lewis, Charles Short, A Latin Dictionary: "A little Pan, a rural deity"[1]
  12. ^ "Chimps and Humans Very Similar at the DNA Level". News.mongabay.com. Retrieved 2009-06-06.
  13. ^ Mary-Claire King (1973) Protein polymorphisms in chimpanzee and human evolution, Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.
  14. ^ Minkel JR (2006-12-19). "Humans and Chimps: Close But Not That Close". Scientific American.
  15. ^ doi: 10.1016/j.ympev.2010.11.005
    This citation will be automatically completed in the next few minutes. You can jump the queue or expand by hand
  16. ^ McBrearty, S.; N. G. Jablonski (2005-09-01). "First fossil chimpanzee". Nature. 437 (7055): 105–8. doi:10.1038/nature04008. PMID 16136135.
  17. ^ Huxley, T. H. (1904). Science and education: Essays. JA Hill and company.
  18. ^ "Chimpanzee", Rolling Hills Wildlife Adventure 2005
  19. ^ "Gene study shows three distinct groups of chimpanzees". EurekAlert. 2007-04-20. Retrieved 2007-04-23.
  20. ^ a b Template:Cite pmid
  21. ^ a b Template:Cite pmid
  22. ^ a b Semendeferi, K; Armstrong, E; Schleicher, A; Zilles, K; Van Hoesen, GW (2001). "Prefrontal cortex in humans and apes: A comparative study of area 10". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 114 (3): 224–41. doi:10.1002/1096-8644(200103)114:3<224::AID-AJPA1022>3.0.CO;2-I. PMID 11241188. Semendeferi et al. using unclear ns of 1–5, report 1,158 cm3 for human brains; chimps have 393 cm3
  23. ^ Tobias, P. (1971). The Brain in Hominid Evolution. New York, Columbia University Press, Template:Hdl; cited in Schoenemann PT. 1997. An MRI study of the relationship between human neuroanatomy and behavioral ability. PhD diss. Univ. of Calif., Berkeley
  24. ^ Schoenemann, P. Thomas (2006). "Evolution of the Size and Functional Areas of the Human Brain". Annual Review of Anthropology. 35 (1): 379–406. doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.35.081705.123210. "Modern human brain sizes vary widely, but average ∼1330 cc (Dekaban 1978, Garby et al. 1993, Ho et al. 1980a, Pakkenberg & Voigt 1964)" these references are listed on this page.
  25. ^ Dekaban AS; Sadowsky, Doris (1978). "Changes in brain weights during the span of human life: relation of brain weights to body heights and body weights". Annals of Neurology. 4 (4): 345–56. doi:10.1002/ana.410040410. PMID 727739.
  26. ^ Garby L, Lammert O, Kock KF, Thobo-Carlsen B (1993). "Weights of brain, liver, kidneys, and spleen in healthy and apparently healthy adult Danish subjects". Am. J. Hum. Biol. 5 (3): 291–96. doi:10.1002/ajhb.1310050307.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  27. ^ Template:Cite pmid
  28. ^ Pakkenberg H, Voigt J (1964). "Brain weight of the Danes". Acta Anatom. 56 (4): 297–307. doi:10.1159/000142510.
  29. ^ Template:Cite pmid
  30. ^ Aiello, L. C. and Dean, M. C. (1990). An introduction to human evolutionary anatomy. London: Academic Press, ISBN 0120455900.
  31. ^ Stephan H, Frahm H, Baron G (1981). "New and Revised Data on Volumes of Brain Structures in Insectivores and Primates". Folia Primatol. 35 (1): 1–29. doi:10.1159/000155963. PMID 7014398.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  32. ^ "Why are rat testicles so big?". ratbehavior.org. 2003–2004. Retrieved 1 September 2009.
  33. ^ "Advanced Age Influences Chimpanzee Behavior in Small Social Groups", Behavior of Aged Chimpanzees
  34. ^ Laird, Courtney (Spring 2004). "Social Organization". Davidson College. Retrieved 2008-03-10.
  35. ^ "Chimp Behavior". Jane Goodall Institute. Archived from the original on 2007-09-27. Retrieved 2007-08-11.
  36. ^ Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'Module:Citation/CS1/Suggestions' not found.
  37. ^ Holland, Jennifer S. (2011). "40 Winks?". National Geographic. 220 (1).
  38. ^ "Chimpanzee intelligence". Indiana University. 2000-02-23. Retrieved 2008-03-24.
  39. ^ Osvath, Mathias (2009-03-10). "Spontaneous planning for future stone throwing by a male chimpanzee". Curr. Biol. 19 (5): R190–1. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.01.010. PMID 19278627.
  40. ^ Mercader J, Barton H, Gillespie J; et al. (2007). "4,300-year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104 (9): 3043–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.0607909104. PMC 1805589. PMID 17360606. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  41. ^ Bijal T. (2004-09-06). "Chimps Shown Using Not Just a Tool but a "Tool Kit"". Retrieved 2010-01-20.
  42. ^ van Lawick-Goodall, Jane. 1970. "Tool-using in Primates and other Vertebrates." Advances in the Study of Behavior, vol. 3, edited by David S. Lehrman, Robert A. Hinde, and Evelyn Shaw. New York: Academic Press.
  43. ^ Fox, M. (2007-02-22). "Hunting chimps may change view of human evolution". Archived from the original on 2007-02-24. Retrieved 2007-02-22.
  44. ^ "ISU anthropologist's study is first to report chimps hunting with tools". Iowa State University News Service. 2007-02-22. Retrieved 2007-08-11.
  45. ^ Carvalho Susana; et al. (2008). "Chaînes Opératoires and resource-exploitation strategies in chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) nut cracking". Journal of Human Evolution. 55 (1): 148–163. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.02.005. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)
  46. ^ Whipps, Heather (2007-02-12). "Chimps Learned Tool Use Long Ago Without Human Help". LiveScience. Retrieved 2007-08-11.
  47. ^ "Tool Use". Jane Goodall Institute. Archived from the original on 2007-05-20. Retrieved 2007-08-11.
  48. ^ Wrangham, Richard W. (1996). Chimpanzee cultures. Chicago Academy of Sciences, Harvard University Press. pp. 115–125. ISBN 978-0-674-11663-4.
  49. ^ Samson DR, Hunt KD (2014). "Chimpanzees Preferentially Select Sleeping Platform Construction Tree Species with Biomechanical Properties that Yield Stable, Firm, but Compliant Nests". PLoS ONE 9(4): e95361. 9 (4): e95361. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0095361. Retrieved 17 April 2014.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  50. ^ "Human-like Altruism Shown In Chimpanzees". Science Daily. 2007-06-25. Retrieved 2007-08-11.
  51. ^ Bradley, Brenda (June 1999). "Levels of Selection, Altruism, and Primate Behavior". The Quarterly Review of Biology. 74 (2): 171–194. doi:10.1086/393070. PMID 10412224.
  52. ^ Silk JB, Brosnan SF, Vonk J; et al. (2005). "Chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of unrelated group members". Nature. 437 (7063): 1357–9. doi:10.1038/nature04243. PMID 16251965. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  53. ^ Boesch, C., Bolé, C.; Eckhardt, N.; Boesch, H. (2010). Santos, Laurie (ed.). "Altruism in Forest Chimpanzees: The Case of Adoption". PLoS ONE. 5 (1): e8901. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0008901.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  54. ^ Harrod, James (May 10, 2007). "Appendices for Chimpanzee Spirituality: A Concise Synthesis of the Literature" (PDF). Retrieved 2011-01-28.
  55. ^ https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=y4Z0xn4pYSY
  56. ^ Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'Module:Citation/CS1/Suggestions' not found.
  57. ^ Walsh, Bryan (2009-02-18). "Why the Stamford Chimp Attacked". TIME. Retrieved 2009-06-06.
  58. ^ Teelen S (2008). "Influence of chimpanzee predation on the red colobus population at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda". Primates. 49 (1): 41–9. doi:10.1007/s10329-007-0062-1. PMID 17906844.
  59. ^ Gibbons A (2007). "Primate behavior. Spear-wielding chimps seen hunting bush babies". Science. 315 (5815): 1063. doi:10.1126/science.315.5815.1063. PMID 17322034.
  60. ^ Pruetz JD, Bertolani P (2007). "Savanna chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus, hunt with tools". Curr. Biol. 17 (5): 412–7. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.12.042. PMID 17320393.
  61. ^ Hockings KJ, Humle T, Anderson JR; et al. (2007). Brosnan, Sarah (ed.). "Chimpanzees share forbidden fruit". PLoS ONE. 2 (9): e886. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000886. PMC 1964537. PMID 17849015. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  62. ^ Richard Gray (24 February 2013). "Chimps solve puzzles for the thrill of it, researchers find". Daily Telegraph. Retrieved 24 February 2013.
  63. ^ Goodall, Jane (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge, Mass.: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-11649-6.
  64. ^ Newswise: Researchers Find Human Virus in Chimpanzees Retrieved on June 5, 2008.
  65. ^ "End chimpanzee research: overview". Project R&R, New England Anti-Vivisection Society. 2005-12-11. Retrieved 2008-03-24.
  66. ^ a b "Chimpanzee lab and sanctuary map". The Humane Society of the United States. Retrieved 2008-03-24.
  67. ^ "Chimpanzee Research: Overview of Research Uses and Costs". Humane Society of the United States. Archived from the original on 2008-03-07. Retrieved 2008-03-24.
  68. ^ a b c d e Lovgren, Stefan. Should Labs Treat Chimps More Like Humans?, National Geographic News, September 6, 2005.
  69. ^ Chimps Deserve Better, Humane Society of the United States.
  70. ^ A former Yerkes lab worker. "Release & Restitution for Chimpanzees in U.S. Laboratories " Wenka". Releasechimps.org. Retrieved 2009-06-06.
  71. ^ Wenka, Project R&R, New England Anti-Vivisection Society.
  72. ^ Langley, Gill (June 2006). Next of Kin: A Report on the Use of Primates in Experiments, British Union for the Abolition of Vivisection, p. 15, citing VandeBerg JL, Zola SM (September 2005). "A unique biomedical resource at risk". Nature. 437 (7055): 30–2. doi:10.1038/437030a. PMID 16136112.
  73. ^ Bradshaw GA, Capaldo T, Lindner L, Grow G (2008). "Building an inner sanctuary: complex PTSD in chimpanzees" (PDF). J Trauma Dissociation. 9 (1): 9–34. doi:10.1080/15299730802073619. PMID 19042307.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  74. ^ Guldberg, Helen. The great ape debate, Spiked online, March 29, 2001. Retrieved August 12, 2007.
  75. ^ Langley, Gill (June 2006). Next of Kin: A Report on the Use of Primates in Experiments, British Union for the Abolition of Vivisection, p. 12.
  76. ^ Gardner, R. A., Gardner, B. T. (1969). "Teaching Sign Language to a Chimpanzee". Science. 165 (3894): 664–672. doi:10.1126/science.165.3894.664. PMID 5793972.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  77. ^ Allen, G. R., Gardner, B. T. (1980). "Comparative psychology and language acquisition". In Thomas A. Sebok and Jean-Umiker-Sebok (eds.) (ed.). Speaking of Apes: A Critical Anthology of Two-Way Communication with Man. New York: Plenum Press. pp. 287–329. ISBN 0306402793. {{cite book}}: |editor= has generic name (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  78. ^ "Language of Bonobos". Great Ape Trust. Archived from the original on 2004-08-15. Retrieved 2012-01-16.
  79. ^ Wynne, Clive (October 31, 2007). "eSkeptic". Skeptic. Retrieved 2011-01-28.
  80. ^ The study was presented in a Channel 5 (UK) documentary "The Memory Chimp", part of the channel's Extraordinary Animals series.
  81. ^ Johnson, Steven (2003-04-01). "Emotions and the Brain". Discover Magazine. Retrieved 2007-12-10.
  82. ^ "Gorilla diary: August – December 2008". BBC News. 2009-01-20. Retrieved 2010-04-28.
  83. ^ "Chimpanzees Don't Make Good Pets". The Jane Goodall Institute. Retrieved 1 February 2015.
  84. ^ a b c d e f g h i Van Riper, A. Bowdoin (2002). Science in popular culture: a reference guide. Westport: Greenwood Press. pp. 18–19. ISBN 0-313-31822-0.
  85. ^ "Animal Actors | PETA.org". Nomoremonkeybusiness.com. Retrieved 2011-01-28.

References

[edit]

Further reading

[edit]
[edit]

Template:Wikispecies

Template:Hominidae nav Template:Apes Template:Phylo Template:Evolution

ln:Mokómbósó nn:Sjimpanse th:ลิงชิมแปนซี