Jump to content

Countershading

This is a good article. Click here for more information.
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
A photograph of a countershading study conducted by the artist Abbott Handerson Thayer, who became obsessed by the mistaken idea that all animals are countershaded.[1] The model on the left is conventionally camouflaged and visible, whereas another on the right is countershaded and invisible.[2]
Ibexes are effectively flattened by countershading, making them nearly invisible against a desert background. There are three in the image.

Countershading, or Thayer's Law, is a form of camouflage. Countershading is the pattern of animal coloration in which an animal’s pigmentation is darker on the upper side and lighter on the underside of the body. This pattern is found in many species of mammals, reptiles, birds, and fish, and has existed since at least the Cretaceous period.

When light falls on a uniformly coloured object such as a sphere from above, it makes the upper side appear lighter and the underside darker, grading from one to the other. This pattern of light and shade makes the object appear solid, and therefore acts as a visual cue which makes the object easier to detect. Countershading reduces the ease of detection of predators and prey by counterbalancing the effects of self-shadowing, again typically with grading from dark to light. In theory this could be useful for military camouflage, but in practice it has rarely been applied, despite the best efforts of the artist Abbott Handerson Thayer and zoologists such as Hugh Cott.

A related mechanism, counter-illumination, adds the creation of light by bioluminescence or lamps to match the actual brightness of a background. Counter-illumination camouflage is common in marine organisms such as squid. It has been studied up to the prototype stage for military use in ships and aircraft, but it too has rarely or never been used in warfare.

The reverse of countershading, with the belly pigmented darker than the back, enhances contrast and so makes animals more conspicuous. It is found in animals that can defend themselves, such as skunks. The pattern is used both in startle or deimatic displays and as a signal to warn off experienced predators. However, animals that habitually live upside-down but lack strong defences, like the Nile catfish and the luna moth caterpillar, have upside-down countershading for camouflage.

Early research

[edit]
Thayer's 1902 patent application. He failed to convince the US Navy.

The English zoologist Edward Bagnall Poulton, author of The Colours of Animals (1890) discovered the countershading of various insects, including the pupa or chrysalis of the purple emperor butterfly, Apatura iris,[3] the caterpillar larvae of the brimstone moth, Opisthograptis luteolata [a] and of the peppered moth, Biston betularia.[b][4][5] However he did not use the term countershading, nor did he suggest that the effect occurred widely.[6]

Thayer's Plate XII in Concealing-Coloration in the Animal Kingdom: Luna Caterpillar Actias luna a) in position b) inverted.
Thayer's "White fowl, lacking counter-shading, against a flat white cloth."

The New Hampshire artist Abbott Handerson Thayer was one of the first to study and write about countershading. In his 1909 book Concealing-Coloration in the Animal Kingdom, he correctly described and illustrated countershading with photographs and paintings, but wrongly claimed that almost all animals are countershaded.[7] For this reason countershading is sometimes called Thayer’s Law. Thayer wrote:

Animals are painted by Nature darkest on those parts which tend to be most lighted by the sky's light, and vice versa. ... the fact that a vast majority of creatures of the whole animal kingdom wear this gradation, developed to an exquisitely minute degree, and are famous for being hard to see in their homes, speaks for itself.

— Thayer[8]

Thayer observed and painted a number of examples, including the Luna moth caterpillar Actias luna, both in its habitual upside-down feeding position, where its countershading makes it appear flat, and artificially inverted from that position, where sunlight and its inverted countershading combine to make it appear heavily shaded and therefore solid.[9] Thayer obtained a patent in 1902 to paint warships, both submarines and surface ships, using countershading,[10] but failed to convince the US Navy to adopt his ideas.[11]

Hugh Bamford Cott in his 1940 book Adaptive Coloration in Animals described many instances of countershading, following Thayer in general approach[12] but criticising Thayer's excessive claim ("He says 'All patterns and colors whatsoever of all animals that ever prey or are preyed upon are under certain normal circumstances obliterative.'") that effectively all animals are camouflaged with countershading. Cott calls this "Thayer straining the theory to a fantastic extreme".[13]

Both Thayer and Cott include in their books photographs of a non-countershaded white cockerel against a white background, to make the point that in Thayer's words "a monochrome object can not be 'obliterated', no matter what its background"[14] or in Cott's words "Colour resemblance alone is not sufficient to afford concealment".[15] Cott explains that

Contrary to what might have been expected by any one lacking in artistic perception, the bird appears highly conspicuous, the back looking lighter, and the breast darker, than the background, although in actual fact, back, background and breast are all pure white."[16]

Application

[edit]

In animals

[edit]
Many marine animals like the grey reef shark are countershaded.

Countershading is observed in a wide range of animal groups, both terrestrial, such as deer, and marine, such as sharks.[17] It is the basis of camouflage in both predators and prey.[18] It is used alongside other forms of camouflage including colour matching and disruptive coloration.[18] Among predatory fish, the gray snapper, Lutianus griseus, is effectively flattened by its countershading, while it hunts an "almost invisible" prey, the hardhead fish, Atherina laticeps which swims over greyish sands.[19] Other countershaded marine animals include blue shark, herring, and dolphin; while fish like the mackerel and sergeant fish are both countershaded and patterned with stripes or spots.[20]

It tones the canvas on which are painted the Leopard's spots, the Tiger's stripes ... It is the dress almost universally worn by rodents... It is the essential uniform adopted by Conies, Asses, Antelopes, Deer ... It is repeated extensively among the marsupials ... It provides a basic livery for the great majority of snakes, lizards, and amphibians. Among insects it reaches a fine state of perfection in different caterpillars and grasshoppers. ... It is, however, in rivers, and in the surface waters of the sea, that countershading reaches its maximum development and significance.

— Hugh Cott[18]

Countershading existed in marine reptiles in the Cretaceous period. Fossilised skin pigmented with dark-coloured eumelanin reveals that both leatherback turtles and mosasaurs had dark backs and light bellies.[21]

[edit]

Another form of animal camouflage uses bioluminescence to increase the average brightness of an animal to match the brightness of the background.[22] This is called counter-illumination. It is common in mid-water pelagic fish and invertebrates especially squid. It makes the counter-illuminated animal practically invisible to predators viewing it from below.[23] As such, counter-illumination camouflage can be seen as an extension beyond what countershading can achieve. Where countershading only paints out shadows, counter-illumination can add in actual lights, permitting effective camouflage in changing conditions, including where the background is bright enough to make an animal that is not counter-illuminated appear as a shadow.[24]

Military

[edit]
Countershaded rail-mounted guns camouflaged by Hugh Cott (above) and in conventional style (below), August 1940. The British authorities agreed Cott's countershading worked, but refused to adopt it.

Countershading, like counter-illumination, has rarely been applied in practice for military camouflage, though not because military authorities were unaware of it. Both Abbott Thayer in the First World War and Hugh Cott in the Second World War proposed countershading to their countries' armed forces. They each demonstrated the effectiveness of countershading, without succeeding in persuading their armed forces to adopt the technique, though they influenced military adoption of camouflage in general.[11]

Cott was a protege of John Graham Kerr who had quarrelled with Norman Wilkinson in the First World War about dazzle camouflage for ships. Wilkinson remained influential in 1939 as an inspector of camouflage, so a political argument developed. Cott was invited to camouflage a 12-inch rail-mounted gun, alongside a similar gun camouflaged conventionally. Cott carefully combined disruptive contrast to break up the gun barrel's outlines with countershading to flatten out its appearance as a solid cylinder. The guns were then photographed from the air from various angles, and in Peter Forbes's view "the results were remarkable."[25] Cott's gun is "invisible except to the most minute scrutiny by someone who knows exactly where to look and what to look for. The other gun is always highly visible." The authorities hesitated, appearing to be embarrassed by the evidence that Cott was right. Cott was posted to the Middle East, and Kerr unsuccessfully intervened, pleading for guns to be painted Cott's way and Cott to be brought home.[26]

A preserved Sherman Firefly (2008); its gun barrel is painted with a pattern that combines countershading and outline disruption to disguise its length.

The Australian zoologist William Dakin in his 1941 book The Art of Camouflage followed Thayer in describing countershading in some detail, and the book was reprinted as a military handbook in 1942. Dakin photographed model birds, much as Thayer and Cott had done, and argued that the shoulders and arms of battledress should be countershaded.[27]

Countershading was described in the US War Department's 1943 Principles of Camouflage, where after four paragraphs of theory and one on its use in nature, the advice given is[28]

Upper surfaces should be painted and textured so as to conform to the color and tone of the surrounding country (background) and the sides graded and toned from this to the white which the under surfaces and parts in shade should be painted.[28]

Photo of WWII fighter plane parked on concrete
Countershaded Focke-Wulf Fw 190D-9

Inventors have continued to advocate military usage of countershading, with for example a 2005 US patent for personal camouflage including countershading in the form of "statistical countercoloring" with varying sizes of rounded dark patches on a lighter ground.[29]

Research by Ariel Tankus and Yehezkel Yeshurun investigating "camouflage breaking", the automated detection of objects such as tanks, showed that analysing images for convexity by looking for graded shadows can "break very strong camouflage, which might delude even human viewers." More precisely, images are searched for places where the gradient of brightness crosses zero, such as the line where a shadow stops becoming darker and starts to become lighter again. The technique defeated camouflage using disruption of edges, but the authors observed that animals with Thayer countershading are using "counter-measures to convexity based detectors", which implied "predators who use convexity based detectors."[30]

Function

[edit]
Countershading acts as a form of camouflage by 'painting out' the self-shadowing of the body or object. The result is a 'flat' appearance, instead of the 'solid' appearance of the body before countershading.

Hannah Rowland, reviewing countershading 100 years after Abbott Thayer, observed that countershading, which she defines as "darker pigmentation on those surfaces exposed to the most lighting" is a common but poorly understood aspect of animal coloration.[6] She noted there had been "much debate" about how countershading works.[31] She considered the evidence for Thayer's theory that this acts as camouflage "by reducing ventral shadowing", and reviewed alternative explanations for countershading.[6]

Camouflage theories of countershading, Rowland wrote, include "self-shadow concealment which results in improved background matching when viewed from the side"; "self-shadow concealment that flattens the form when viewed from the side"; "background matching when viewed from above or below"; and "body outline obliteration when viewed from above".[6] These are examined in turn below.

Flattening and background matching when viewed from the side

[edit]
Squirrel oriented horizontally
When oriented horizontally, the countershading of the gray squirrel, Sciurus carolinensis, helps to "paint out" its ventral shadow.
Squirrel oriented vertically
When oriented vertically, the gray squirrel's pale belly is conspicuous rather than camouflaged.

Cott, like Thayer, argued that countershading would make animals hard to see from the side, as they would "fade into a ghostly elusiveness".[32] Rowland notes that Cott is here reviewing Thayer's theory and "reinforcing the view that a gradation in shading would act to eliminate the effects of ventral shadowing."[6] Kiltie measured the effect of the countershading of the grey squirrel, Sciurus carolinensis, showing that when the squirrel is horizontal the self-shadowing of the belly is partly concealed, but that when the squirrel is vertical (as when climbing a tree trunk) this effect did not occur.[33]

Thayer's original argument, restated by Cott,[32] was that nature did the exact opposite with countershading that an artist did with paint when creating the illusion of solid three-dimensionality, namely counteracting the effect of shade to flatten out form. Shading is a powerful cue used by animals in different phyla to identify the shapes of objects. Research with chicks showed that they preferred to peck at grains with shadows falling below them (as if illuminated from above), so both humans and birds may make use of shading as a depth cue.[6][34]

Background matching from above or below

[edit]
The mackerel, Scomber scombrus, like many pelagic fish, is dark above, pale below, camouflaging it against the ocean depths and the bright surface.

Among pelagic fish it is common to find the upper surface dark-coloured and the lower surface white, so that the animal is inconspicuous when seen either from above or below.

— Frank Evers Beddard[35]

Early researchers including Alfred Russel Wallace,[36] Frank Evers Beddard,[37] Cott[38] and Craik[39] argued that marine animals including pelagic fish such as marlin and mackerel, as well as dolphins, sharks, and penguins the upper and lower surfaces are sharply distinct in tone, with a dark upper surface and often a nearly white lower surface. They suggested that when seen from the top, the darker dorsal surface of the animal would offer camouflage against the darkness of the deep water below. When seen from below, the lighter ventral area would similarly provide the least possible contrast with the sunlit ocean surface above.[6] There is some evidence for this in birds, where birds that catch fish at a medium depth, rather than at the surface or on the seabed, are more often countershaded, and the prey of these birds would see only the underside of the bird.[40] Rowland concluded that each possible role for countershading needs to be evaluated separately, rather than just assuming it functions effectively.[6]

Outline obliteration from above

[edit]

Rowland (2009) identified an additional mechanism of countershading not previously analysed, namely that a round body like a cylinder illuminated and seen from above appears to have dark sides. Using a graphics tool, she demonstrated that this effect can be flattened out by countershading. Since predators are known to use edges to identify prey, countershading may therefore, she argues, make prey harder to detect when seen from above.[6]

Non-camouflage theories

[edit]

Non-camouflage theories include protection from ultraviolet light; thermoregulation; and protection from abrasion. All three of these "plausible" theories remained largely untested in 2009, according to Rowland.[6]

Evidence

[edit]
The sea slug Glaucus atlanticus swims and is countershaded upside-down.

Despite demonstrations and examples adduced by Cott and others, little experimental evidence for the effectiveness of countershading was gathered in the century since Thayer's discovery. Experiments in 2009 using artificial prey showed that countershaded objects do have survival benefits[41] and in 2012, a study by William Allen and colleagues showed that countershading in 114 species of ruminants closely matched predictions for "self-shadow concealment", the function predicted by Poulton, Thayer and Cott.[42]

Mechanism

[edit]

Evolutionary developmental biology ('Evo Devo') has assembled evidence from embryology and genetics to show how evolution has acted at all scales from the whole organism down to individual genes, proteins and genetic switches. In the case of countershaded mammals with dark (often brownish) upper parts and lighter (often buff or whitish) under parts, such as in the house mouse, it is the Agouti gene which creates the difference in shading. Agouti encodes for a protein, the Agouti signalling peptide (ASP), which specifically inhibits the action of the Melanocortin 1 receptor (MC1R). In the absence of the Agouti protein, alpha-melanocyte-stimulating hormone stimulates the cells bearing MC1R, melanocytes, to produce dark eumelanin, colouring the skin and fur dark brown or black. In the presence of the Agouti protein, the same system produces the lighter-coloured, yellow or red phaeomelanin. A genetic switch active in the cells of the embryo that will become the belly skin causes the Agouti gene to become active there, creating the countershading seen in adult mammals.[43]

Reverse countershading

[edit]
The honey badger is reverse countershaded, a form of warning coloration.

If countershading paints out shadows, the reverse, darkening the belly and lightening the back, would maximise contrast by adding to the natural fall of light. This pattern of animal coloration is found in animals like the skunk and honey badger with strong defences—the offensive stink of the skunk, and the sharp claws, aggressive nature and stink of the honey badger.[44] These animals do not run when under attack, but move slowly, often turning to face the danger, and giving deimatic or threat displays either to startle inexperienced predators, or as an aposematic signal, to warn off experienced ones.[45]

The caterpillar of the Luna moth, as discovered by Thayer, is in Cott's phrase "countershaded in relation to [its] attitude", i.e. shaded with a light back grading to a dark belly, as also is the Nile catfish, Synodontis batensoda for the same reason: these animals (and other caterpillars including Automeris io and the eyed hawkmoth, Smerinthus ocellatus) habitually live 'upside down' with the belly uppermost. Similarly in the sea slug Glaucus atlanticus, the reverse countershading is associated with inverted habits. These animals are thus employing countershading in the usual way for camouflage.[46]

Examples in animals

[edit]

See also

[edit]

Notes

[edit]
  1. ^ It was called Rumia crataegata at the time.
  2. ^ It was called Amphidasis betularia at the time.

References

[edit]
  1. ^ Forbes, 2009. pp. 76–79.
  2. ^ Behrens, 2009.
  3. ^ Poulton, 1888.
  4. ^ Poulton, 1887.
  5. ^ Thayer, 1909. p 22.
  6. ^ a b c d e f g h i j Rowland, 2009.
  7. ^ Thayer, 1909.
  8. ^ Thayer, 1909. pp 14–15.
  9. ^ Thayer, 1909. Plate XII.
  10. ^ Template:US Patent
  11. ^ a b Goldstein, 2009, pp. 233–235.
  12. ^ Cott, 1940. pp. 35–47.
  13. ^ Cott, 1940. pp. 172–173.
  14. ^ Thayer, 1909. Caption to Figure 7.
  15. ^ Cott, 1940. Caption to Plate 7.
  16. ^ Cott, 1940. p. 35.
  17. ^ ONR, 2013.
  18. ^ a b c Cott, 1940. p. 40.
  19. ^ Cott, 1940. p37.
  20. ^ Cott, 1940. p41
  21. ^ Lindgren, Johan and Peter Sjövall, Ryan M. Carney, Per Uvdal, Johan A. Gren, Gareth Dyke, Bo Pagh Schultz, Matthew D. Shawkey, Kenneth R. Barnes, Michael J. Polcyn (February 2014). "Skin pigmentation provides evidence of convergent melanism in extinct marine reptiles". Nature. 506 (7489): 484–488. doi:10.1038/nature12899. PMID 24402224.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  22. ^ Young and Roper, 1977.
  23. ^ Young and Roper, 1976.
  24. ^ Jones, 2004. p. 1151.
  25. ^ Forbes, 2009. p. 150.
  26. ^ Forbes, 2009. pp. 142–146, 149-151, 156.
  27. ^ Elias, 2011.
  28. ^ a b Anon, 1943.
  29. ^ Tooley, 2005.
  30. ^ Tankus and Yeshurun, 2013.
  31. ^ Rowland, 2011.
  32. ^ a b Cott, 1940. pp 36–37.
  33. ^ Kiltie, 1944.
  34. ^ Hershberger, 1970.
  35. ^ Beddard, 1892. p. 122.
  36. ^ Wallace, 1889, p 193
  37. ^ Beddard, 1895, p 115
  38. ^ Cott, 1940.
  39. ^ Craik, 1944.
  40. ^ Ruxton 2004.
  41. ^ Rowland et al, 2009.
  42. ^ Allen et al, 2012.
  43. ^ Carroll, Sean B. (2006). Endless Forms Most Beautiful. Weidenfeld and Nicolson. pp. 229–231, 237. ISBN 978-0-297-85094-6.
  44. ^ "Black, White and Stinky: Explaining Coloration in Skunks and Other Boldly Colored Animals". University of Massachusetts Amherst. 27 May 2011. Retrieved 19 June 2014.
  45. ^ Edmunds, 2008.
  46. ^ Cott, 1940. p. 43.

Bibliography

[edit]

Pioneering books

[edit]

General reading

[edit]
  • Behrens, Roy R (2009). Goldstein, E Bruce (ed.). Encyclopedia of Perception, Volume 1. Sage. pp. 233–235.
  • Edmunds, Malcolm (2008). "Deimatic Behavior". In Capinera, John L (ed.). Encyclopedia of Entomology. Springer.
  • Forbes, Peter (2009). Dazzled and Deceived: Mimicry and Camouflage. Yale.
  • Rowland, Hannah M (2011). "The history, theory and evidence for a cryptic function of countershading". In Stevens, Martin; Merilaita, Sami (ed.). Animal Camouflage: Mechanisms and Function. Cambridge University Press.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: editors list (link)
  • Ruxton, Graeme D; Sherratt, Tom N; Speed, Michael P (2004). "3. Countershading and counterillumination". Avoiding Attack: The Evolutionary Ecology of Crypsis, Warning Signals and Mimicry. Oxford University Press.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)

Journals

[edit]

Template:Camouflage Template:Evo ecol Template:Vision in animals